Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-sxzjt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-19T06:04:06.238Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

The brainstem and mental health tribunals in Scotland

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Frank Corrigan*
Affiliation:
Argyll and Bute Hospital, Lochgilphead, Argyll PA31 8LD, email: frank.corrigan@nhs.net
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type
The columns
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2009

In an elegant and important brain imaging study of response to threat, Mobbs et al (Reference Mobbs, Petrovic and Marchant2007) demonstrated that increasing proximity of a virtual predator shifts human brain activation from the prefrontal cortex to the midbrain periaqueductal gray - the area responsible for active and passive defence responses (Reference Bandler, Keay and FloydBandler et al, 2000).

In a similar paradigm, Butler et al (Reference Butler, Pan and Tuescher2007) confirmed that feeling under threat, compared with feeling safe, is characterised by a shift from cortical to subcortical activation.

As there is a simultaneous reduction in activity in the areas associated with emotional autobiographical memory, such as hippocampus and precuneus, emotionally charged memories of dynamic motor sequences (Reference McHaffie, Stanford and SteinMcHaffie et al, 2005) may be more accessible to body-oriented approaches than to verbal episode-recall therapies (e.g. Reference Ogden, Minton and PainOgden et al, 2006). When such pronounced effects are evident with virtual reality or laboratory paradigms, it is almost certain that they are much more active in situations of real threat, whether physical, social or professional.

At a recent mental health tribunal for Scotland hearing, I witnessed a patient's solicitor employing more and more threatening postural and verbal behaviours in response to the difficulties a social work colleague (mental health officer) was experiencing in keeping language and working-memory areas of the brain active in the face of the solicitor's ruthlessly adversarial approach. As would be predicted from the imaging studies, the increase in threat experienced during this savaging resulted in an even more reduced ability to access prefrontal functioning, with the result that the colleague's evidence was then easily and contemptuously dismissed (‘with the greatest respect’, of course, in the legal-speak for which clinical training provides no translation ability). Shame, the toxic and contagious emotion (Reference MollonMollon, 1996) accompanying humiliated submission to predatory agonistic dominance (also a periaqueductal gray-mediated function (Reference Haller, Tóth and HalaszHaller et al, 2006) neurobiologically incompatible with nurturing in the rat (Reference Sukikara, Mota-Ortiz and BaldoSukikara et al, 2006)) incorporates an urge towards social exclusion, which can be mirrored in the distancing of observers from the views of the victim. In other words, there is no need to wait for a decision based on points, if the contest has been decided by a knockout and a thrown towel: a victory for the judicial process.

It may be argued that those whose motivational drive switches from seeking or appetitive mode to fearful mode when the environment becomes hostile and the valence of the nucleus accumbens activation rapidly changes (Reference Reynolds and BerridgeReynolds & Berridge, 2008) should leave the heat of their occupational kitchen. This in itself increases the threat status of the situation and makes it potentially more damaging in the long term for the mental health professional who, for reasons of professional pride, will find it impossible to see the situation as other than inescapable.

Although such a situation is nowhere near as traumatising as physical or sexual assault, the combination of an intense fear, a feeling of helplessness and an inability to escape are the features which characterise traumatic experiences, with the potential to cause long-term psychological damage (Reference Bovin, Jager-Hyman and GoldBovin et al, 2008). Any previous experience of trauma-induced helplessness, from which mental health professionals are not immune, will compound the damage through triggering of body memories and intensification of the distress.

From a neurobiological perspective, it is my contention that my colleague was being subjected to a traumatising experience with a potential for long-term damage.

Employers and professional bodies, including the Royal College of Psychiatrists, which facilitate such confrontations and condone the legislation which requires them, in contrast to the careful and respectful collection of data and information during cross-examination before a sheriff which happened under the old Act (1984), will presumably employ the nucleus accumbens/kitchen exit argument.

However, mental health professionals who are not trained as lawyers and are therefore bound to be losers in any such agonistic encounter may in due course be able to claim before tribunals of the employment variety that they are being subjected to unnecessary traumatisation by their employers.

What was the outcome of the tribunal hearing? There was a stunningly ingenious display of an ability to turn a straightforward question requiring a clear answer into a fence-sitting compromise which is likely to require another series of hearings within a few months. This is probably bad news for the patient, as this tribunal intimated that long-term detention would be appropriate but, of course, hearings are very convener-specific and unpredictable, so the hopes of the patient may not be being raised only to be dashed. It is certainly bad news for the mental health staff that will be exposed again to unnecessary vilification, by people who have no clinical experience or understanding of the complexities of a duty of care, to the detriment of the therapeutic relationship with the patient. However, to end on a happier note, it is great news for the lawyers in this tribunal industry which has a remarkable capacity for maximising the number of hearings that is required.

References

Bandler, R., Keay, K. A., Floyd, N., et al (2000) Central circuits mediating patterned autonomic activity during active vs. passive emotional coping. Brain Research Bulletin, 53, 95104.Google Scholar
Bovin, M. J., Jager-Hyman, S., Gold, S. D., et al (2008) Tonic immobility mediates the influence of peritraumatic fear and perceived inescapability on posttraumatic stress symptom severity among sexual assault survivors. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 21, 402409.Google Scholar
Butler, T., Pan, H., Tuescher, O., et al (2007) Human fear-related motor neurocircuitry. Neuroscience, 150, 17.Google Scholar
Haller, J., Tóth, M., Halasz, J., et al (2006) Patterns of violent aggression-induced brain c-fos expression in male mice selected for aggressiveness. Physiology and Behavior, 88, 173182.Google Scholar
McHaffie, J. G., Stanford, T. R., Stein, B. E., et al (2005) Subcortical loops through the basal ganglia. Trends in Neurosciences, 28, 401407.Google Scholar
Mobbs, D., Petrovic, P., Marchant, J. L., et al (2007) When fear is near: threat imminence elicits prefrontal-periaqueductal gray shifts in humans. Science, 317, 10791083.Google Scholar
Mollon, P. (1996) Multiple Selves, Multiple Voices: Working with Trauma, Violation, and Dissociation. John Wiley.Google Scholar
Ogden, P., Minton, K. & Pain, C. (2006) Trauma and the Body: A Sensorimotor Approach to Psychotherapy. Norton.Google Scholar
Reynolds, S. M. & Berridge, K. C. (2008) Emotional environments retune the valence of appetitive versus fearful functions in nucleus accumbens. Nature Neuroscience, 11, 423425.Google Scholar
Sukikara, M. H., Mota-Ortiz, S. R., Baldo, M.V., et al (2006) A role for the periaqueductal gray in switching adaptive behavioural responses. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 25832589.Google Scholar
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.