Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-nwzlb Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-28T16:10:44.338Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Effectiveness and confusion of the Time to Change anti-stigma campaign

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 January 2018

Abu Abraham
Affiliation:
Basildon Hospital
Joby M. Easow
Affiliation:
Community Drug and Alcohol Service, Pitsea, Basildon
Palanisamy Ravichandren
Affiliation:
Runwell Hospital, Wickford
Salman Mushtaq
Affiliation:
Taylor Centre, Southend-on-Sea, UK
Linda Butterworth
Affiliation:
Taylor Centre, Southend-on-Sea, UK
Jason Luty*
Affiliation:
Taylor Centre, Southend-on-Sea, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Aims and method

Several national anti-stigma campaigns have been devised in the UK, including the current Time to Change campaign in England. Our aim was to assess whether the campaign promotional materials were likely to have any effect on public attitudes towards mental illness. Postcards, leaflets and bookmarks promoting the campaign were posted to 250 participants recruited from a representative panel of members of the public. Two weeks later a questionnaire was sent to assess the impact the campaign materials had.

Results

The response rate was 78%. Only 23% of participants recognised the Time to Change logo after 2 weeks and only 20% correctly reported that one in four people were affected by mental health problems when presented with five alternative responses. Almost as many participants thought the campaign was promoting a British political party rather than discrimination against mental illness.

Clinical implications

A single exposure to Time to Change campaign materials is unlikely to be effective. The title of the campaign is likely to be confused with political campaigning in Britain.

Type
Original Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution (CC-BY) license (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited.
Copyright
Copyright © The Royal College of Psychiatrists, 2010

Stigma is a social construction that devalues people because of a distinguishing characteristic or mark. Reference Biernat, Dovidio, Heatherton, Kleck and Hebl1 The World Health Organization and the World Psychiatric Association recognise that the stigma attached to mental disorders is strongly associated with suffering, disability and poverty. Reference Corrigan, Markowitz, Watson, Rowan and Kubiak2 Stigma is also a major barrier to seeking treatment. Reference Appleby3 Many studies show that negative attitudes towards the mentally ill are widespread, while the media generally depict mentally ill people as violent, erratic and dangerous. Reference Granello, Pauley and Carmichael4,Reference Crisp, Gelder, Rix, Meltzer and Rowlands5

Providing factual information in brief fact sheets Reference Penn, Guynan, Daily, Spaulding, Garbin and Sullivan6-Reference Penn, Kommana, Mansfield and Link8 or through extensive interventions such as educational courses is reported to reduce stigma. Reference Thornton and Wahl9-Reference Mayville and Penn11 These methods have been the basis of several anti-stigma campaigns. For example, the Royal College of Psychiatrists' Changing Minds campaign aimed to promote positive images of mental illness, challenge misrepresentations and discrimination, and educate the public to the real nature and treatability of mental disorder. Reference Crisp, Gelder, Rix, Meltzer and Rowlands5 This campaign contributed towards Action on Mental Health: A Guide to Promoting Social Inclusion, a document of 12 fact sheets on reducing the stigma of mental illness, with practical advice to health agencies, employers and stakeholders. Reference Penn and Martin12 More recently, national anti-stigma campaigns have been launched in Scotland (www.seemescotland.org.uk) and England (www.time-to-change.org.uk). Unfortunately, there have been reports that national anti-stigma campaigns are not particularly effective. Reference Corrigan and Penn13-Reference Paykel, Hart and Priest15 These reports discuss the disappointing results from the English Defeat Depression and Changing Minds campaigns and the (Scottish) See Me campaign.

We wanted to assess whether promotional materials from the Time to Change campaign (postcards, leaflets and bookmarks) were likely to have any effect on public attitudes towards mental illness.

Method

Participants

We identified a panel of 250 participants from the UK general population recruited using direct mail-shots and adverts in local newspapers as described in a previous study. Reference Luty, Umoh, Sessay and Sarkhel16

Procedure

Each participant was posted a Time to Change postcard, leaflet and bookmark, where it was clearly stated that mental health problems affect ‘1 in 4 people’. Two weeks later a questionnaire was posted out (see online supplement to this article). The attitude of participants towards mental illness was tested using the 5-item Attitude to Mental Illness Questionnaire (AMIQ), with a vignette describing a man with depression and an episode of self-harm. The questionnaire also enquired about participants' gender, age, ethnicity and employment status, recognition of the Time to Change logo, awareness of the ‘1 in 4’ slogan and the purpose of the Time to Change campaign.

Instruments

The 5-item AMIQ is a brief self-completion questionnaire. Reference Luty, Umoh, Sessay and Sarkhel16,Reference Mehta, Kassam, Leese, Butler and Thornicroft17 Respondents read a short vignette describing an imaginary patient and answered five questions relating to it (online supplement). The questions were scored on a 5-point Likert scale (maximum + 2, minimum – 2), with blank questions, ‘neutral’ and ‘don't know’ scoring zero. The total score for the vignette ranged between – 10 and + 10. The AMIQ has been shown to have good psychometric properties in a sample of over 800 members of the UK general public (one component accounted for 80.2% of the variance; test-retest reliability was P = 0.702; alternate test-reliability v. Corrigan's attribution questionnaire was 0.704 (Spearman's Rho); Cronbach's α = 0.93). Reference Luty, Umoh, Sessay and Sarkhel16

Results

Completed questionnaires were received from 196 individuals (response rate 78%); 32% were male; mean age was 50 years (s.e. = 1.1); 56% were in paid employment; 92% described themselves as White British; 41% endorsed the item ‘Do you know anyone personally who has a serious mental illness (like schizophrenia) or someone who has been in a mental hospital?’

The mean score was +1.92 (s.e. = 0.29; s.d. = 2.91) for the 5-point AMIQ stigma scale. We compared this with a score from the AMIQ validation 3 years earlier on 1098 members of the UK general public: + 2.35 (s.e. = 0.10). Reference Luty, Umoh, Sessay and Sarkhel16 There was no statistically significant difference in scores (Mann-Whitney U-test two-sided P = 0.0367; power (for 5% significance) = 55%).

Campaign recognition

The campaign logo was recognised by 23% of participants. However, only 17% stated that they had ever heard of the Time to Change campaign. Only 20% correctly reported that one in four people were affected by mental health problems when presented with five alternative responses (33% chose one in three; 14% one in five; 12% one in six; and 34% did not know). When asked ‘What issue or organisation does the Time to Change campaign promote?’ and presented with six alternatives, 24% correctly identified ‘discrimination against mental illness’. However, 57% endorsed ‘Don't know/None of the above,’ and 20% endorsed ‘The Liberal Party’.

Familiarity with mental illness

Overall, 82 (42%) participants had contact with a mentally ill person. Still, familiarity with mental illness had no significant effect on the AMIQ stigma scores or familiarity with the campaign. The 45 participants who recognised the campaign logo had a significantly increased chance of correctly identifying the objective of the campaign (Yates-corrected χ2 = 6.95; P = 0.0084; OR = 2.74 (95% CI 1.34-5.6)), but they had a reduced chance of identifying the ‘1 in 4’ catchphrase (Yates-corrected χ2 = 0.02; P = 0.8983; OR = 0.88 (95% CI 0.4-1.93)).

Discussion

A single exposure to three forms of promotional materials from the Time to Change anti-stigma campaign (postcards, leaflets and bookmarks) had little discernable effect on public attitudes towards mental illness. There was no significant change on scores of the AMIQ when performed in respect to a fictitious man with depression who self-harmed. Only a minority of participants (less than 25%) recognised the logo, the name of the campaign, the ‘1 in 4’ message and the campaign objective. This was despite the fact that three brief items of campaign materials had been posted directly to participants only 14 days before the questionnaire. There also seemed to be significant confusion between the anti-stigma Time to Change campaign and campaigns by British political parties. For example, the Conservative Party is currently using several variations on the ‘time to change’ themes in its marketing - ‘vote to change’, ‘election to change’; online Fig. DS1). This was topical at the time of the survey (April 2009) in the run up to the European Parliament elections in June that year. A similar message is being promoted by most British political parties, including the Liberal Democrats. Almost as many participants believed that the Time to Change campaign was being used to promote the ‘Liberal Party’ and that its objective was to reduce discrimination towards mental illness (20% v. 24%). As the political campaigns are likely to intensify rather than diminish in the run up to the next UK general election (which must take place on or before June 2010), this is likely to increase the confusion and reduce the effectiveness of the Time to Change anti-stigma campaign.

A second problem of the Time to Change campaign is common to all promotional campaigns, including those of political parties and commercial product advertising. The general public is saturated with promotional literature and it is extremely difficult to get noticed and transmit any message. Reference Luty, Fekadu, Umoh and Gallagher18,Reference Cunningham, Sobell and Chow19 Even factual stories in newspapers have little impact (e.g. on voting behaviour). Consequently, marketing campaigns and newspapers tend to associate their products or stories with publicly recognised celebrities. Reference Luty, Fekadu, Umoh and Gallagher18,Reference Wilmshurst and Mackay20 Although the Time to Change campaign is endorsed and promoted publicly by celebrities such as Stephen Fry and Ruby Wax, they were not present on the materials used in our study.

It is unlikely that a single exposure to Time to Change materials would be effective. Although attempting to ensure repeated regular exposure over many months is prohibitively expensive, this is likely to be the only means of effecting significant ‘market penetration’ and attitude change.

There are now many suggested means of reducing the stigma of mental illness, including Action on Mental Health Reference Penn and Martin12 and Changing Minds. 14 However, responses to these campaigns tend to be small, especially if negative consequences of mental illness are also disseminated. Reference Jones, Kavanagh, Moran and Norton21 Pinfold et al Reference Atkinson, Atkinson, Smith, Bem and Nolen-Hoeksema22 reported a project in which 472 English secondary school children attended mental health awareness workshops. Knox et al Reference Penn, Chamberlin and Mueser23 studied addressing stigmatised attitudes to mental illness among 4 million members of the US armed forces, which involved mandatory training on recognition of mental illness. Although this significantly reduced suicide rates, there was no effect on stigmatised attitudes. It was possible in a military or secondary school setting to insist on engagement in anti-stigma training. By contrast, involvement of the general public in any campaign is entirely voluntary.

Promoting direct interpersonal contact with people who are mentally ill may be an effective strategy, but the amount of contact required remains unknown. Reference Penn, Guynan, Daily, Spaulding, Garbin and Sullivan6,Reference Mayville and Penn11,Reference Penn, Chamberlin and Mueser23,Reference Pinfold, Toulmin, Thornicroft, Huxley, Farmer and Graham24 There was no significant effect of personal familiarity with a mentally ill person and stigmatised attitudes in our study, despite the fact that almost half of the respondents reported some familiarity with mental illness. This was also noted in an earlier study which showed no difference in the attitude of participants to people with alcoholism and those with opiate dependence, despite the fact that alcohol problems are several times more common in Britain than opiate dependence. Reference Knox, Smith and Hereby25 This argues against the anti-stigmatising effect of direct contact with people with certain mental illnesses. Furthermore, it would be difficult in practice to ensure that a significant proportion of the public had contact with people with a severe mental illness.

Study strengths and limitations

The AMIQ was used in this study as it is convenient and has been well validated. Reference Luty, Umoh, Sessay and Sarkhel16,Reference Mehta, Kassam, Leese, Butler and Thornicroft17,Reference Knox, Smith and Hereby25 Other instruments are available, although they tend to be much longer, involve interviews or tend to address the experience of stigma by people with mental illness themselves (e.g. the Internalised Stigma of Mental Illness scale). Reference Luty, Umoh, Sessay and Sarkhel16,Reference Wolff, Pathare, Craig and Leff27

Although there were more female respondents, age and employment status of participants were reasonably matched to that from UK census surveys and the sample appears to be a reasonable cross-section of the British public. However, the sample was self-selecting and may not generalise across the whole population. Ideally, interviews could be conducted using a quota survey of households with repeat visits for non-responders. Reference Crisp, Gelder, Rix, Meltzer and Rowlands5 Unfortunately, this is prohibitively expensive.

The study presented a hypothetical person who was mentally ill. This is less accurate than real experience. Moreover, the written views and expressed attitudes may not translate into any enduring behavioural change. It was not possible to measure stigmatised behaviour towards real people who are mentally ill.

There was no direct contact between participants and researchers, but participants are likely to make some assumptions about the potentially liberal beliefs of researchers into mental health. Hence social desirability bias may affect the results. However, the results from previous studies using the AMIQ suggest that participants had little reservation about indicating their disapproval of people with stigmatising illnesses such as substance use disorders. Reference Luty, Umoh, Sessay and Sarkhel16,Reference Knox, Smith and Hereby25 This would indicate that social disability bias has only a modest effect.

Footnotes

Declaration of interest

None.

References

1 Biernat, M, Dovidio, JF. Stigma and stereotypes. In The Social Psychology of Stigma (eds Heatherton, TF, Kleck, RE, Hebl, MR): 88125. Guilford Press,, 2000.Google Scholar
2 Corrigan, P, Markowitz, FE, Watson, A, Rowan, D, Kubiak, MA. An attribution model of public discrimination towards people with mental illness. J Health Soc Behav 2003; 44: 162–79.Google Scholar
3 Appleby, L. Safer Services. Department of Health, 1999.Google Scholar
4 Granello, D, Pauley, PS, Carmichael, A. Relationship of the media to attitudes towards people with mental illness. J Consult Psychol Counsel Educ Develop 1999; 38: 89110.Google Scholar
5 Crisp, AH, Gelder, MG, Rix, S, Meltzer, HI, Rowlands, OJ. Stigmatisation of people with mental illnesses. Br J Psychiatry 2000; 177: 47.Google Scholar
6 Penn, DL, Guynan, K, Daily, T, Spaulding, WD, Garbin, CP, Sullivan, M. Dispelling the stigma of schizophrenia: what sort of information is best? Schizophr Bull 1994; 20: 567–77.Google Scholar
7 Penn, DL, Kommana, S, Mansfield, M, Link, BG. Dispelling the stigma of schizophrenia: 2. The impact of information on dangerousness. Schizophr Bull 1999; 25, 437–46.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
8 Thornton, JA, Wahl, OF. Impact of a newspaper article on attitudes toward mental illness. J Community Psychol 1996; 24: 1725.Google Scholar
9 Mayville, E, Penn, DL. Changing societal attitudes towards persons with severe mental illness. Cogn Behav Practice 1998; 5: 241–53.Google Scholar
10 Penn, DL, Martin, J. The stigma of severe mental illness. Some potential solutions for a recalcitrant problem. Psychiatr Q 1998; 69: 235–47.Google Scholar
11 Corrigan, PW, Penn, DL. Lessons from social psychology on discrediting psychiatric stigma. Am Psychol 1999; 54: 765–76.Google Scholar
12 Office of the Deputy Prime Minister. Action on Mental Health: A Guide to Promoting Social Inclusion. ODPM, 2004 (http://www.socialinclusion.org.uk/publications/Action_on_Mental_Health%20Fact_Sheets.pdf).Google Scholar
13 Paykel, ES, Hart, D, Priest, RG. Changes in public attitudes to depression during the Defeat Depression Campaign. Br J Psychiatry 1998; 173: 519–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
14 Luty, J, Umoh, O, Sessay, M, Sarkhel, A. Effectiveness of Changing Minds campaign factsheets in reducing stigmatised attitudes towards mental illness. Psychiatr Bull 2007; 31: 377–81.Google Scholar
15 Mehta, N, Kassam, A, Leese, M, Butler, G, Thornicroft, G. Public attitudes towards people with mental illness in England and Scotland, 1994–2003. Br J Psychiatry 2009; 194: 278–84.Google Scholar
16 Luty, J, Fekadu, D, Umoh, O, Gallagher, J. Validation of a short instrument to measure stigmatised attitudes towards mental illness. Psychiatr Bull 2006; 30: 257–60.Google Scholar
17 Cunningham, JA, Sobell, LC, Chow, VMC. What's in a label? The effects of substance types and labels on treatment considerations and stigma. J Stud Alcohol 1993; 54: 693–9.Google Scholar
18 Wilmshurst, J, Mackay, A. The Fundamentals and Practice of Marketing (4th edn). Butterworth, 2002.Google Scholar
19 Jones, B, Kavanagh, D, Moran, M, Norton, P. Politics UK (6th edn). Pearson, 2007.Google Scholar
20 Atkinson, RL, Atkinson, RC, Smith, EE, Bem, DJ, Nolen-Hoeksema, S. Hilgard's Introduction to Psychology (12th edn). Harcourt Brace, 1996.Google Scholar
21 Penn, DL, Chamberlin, C, Mueser, KT. The effects of a documentary film about schizophrenia on psychiatric stigma. Schizophr Bull 2003; 29: 383–91.Google Scholar
22 Pinfold, V, Toulmin, H, Thornicroft, G, Huxley, P, Farmer, P, Graham, T. Reducing psychiatric stigma and discrimination: evaluation of interventions in UK secondary schools. Br J Psychiatry 2003; 182: 342–6.Google Scholar
23 Knox, T, Smith, J, Hereby, H. Risk of suicide and related adverse outcomes after exposure to a suicide prevention programme in the US air force: cohort study. BMJ 2003; 327: 1376–8.Google Scholar
24 Wolff, G, Pathare, S, Craig, T, Leff, J. Public education for community care. A new approach. Br J Psychiatry 1996; 168: 441–7.Google Scholar
25 Luty, J, Rao, H, Arokiadass, SMR, Easow, JM, Sarkhel, A. The repentant sinner: methods to reduce stigmatised attitudes towards mental illness. Psychiatr Bull 2008; 32: 327–32.Google Scholar
26 Crisp, AH, Gelder, M, Goddard, E, Meltzer, H. Stigmatisation of people with mental illness: a follow-up study within the Changing Minds campaign of the Royal College of Psychiatrists. World Psychiatry 2005; 4: 106–13.Google Scholar
27 Ritsher, JB, Otilingam, PG, Grajales, M. Internalised stigma of mental illness: psychometric properties of a new measure. Psychiatry Res 2003; 121: 3149.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Abraham et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Material

Download Abraham et al. supplementary material(File)
File 437 Bytes
Supplementary material: PDF

Abraham et al. supplementary material

Supplementary Material

Download Abraham et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 72.3 KB
Submit a response

eLetters

No eLetters have been published for this article.